Contributions of cognitive neuropsychological research Brenda Rapp and Matthew Goldrick
We review the significant cognitive neuropsychological contributions to our
understanding of spoken word production that were made during the period of 1984 to 2004-since the founding of the journal Cognitive Neuropsychology. We then go on to identify and
discuss a set of outstanding questions and challenges that face future cognitive
neuropsychological researchers in this domain. We conclude that the last twenty years have been
a testament to the vitality and productiveness of this approach in the domain of spoken word
production and that it is essential that we continue to strive for the broader integration of
cognitive neuropsychological evidence into cognitive science, psychology, linguistics and
neuroscience.
& Marin, O. S. M. (1980). Fractionating the reading process in dementia: Evidence for word-specific print-to-sound associations. In M. Coltheart, K. E. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.) Deep dyslexia (pp. 259-269). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., Bloch, D. E., & Dell, G. S. (1994). Disordered speech production in aphasic and normal speakers. Brain and Language, 47, 52-88. Schwartz, M. F., Wilshire, C. E., Gagnon, D. A., & Polansky, M. (2004). Origins of nonword phonological errors in aphasic picture naming. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 159-186. Semenza, C., Butterworth, B., Panzeri, M., & Ferreri, T. (1990). Word formation: New evidence from aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 28, 499-502. Semenza, C., & Sgaramella, T. (1993), Production of proper names: A clinical study of the effects of phonemic cueing. Memory, 1, 265-228. Semenza, C. & Zettin, M. (1988). Generating proper names: A case of selective inability. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 711-772. Semenza, C. & Zettin, M. (1989). Evidence from aphasia for the role of proper names as pure referring expressions. Nature, 342, 678–679 Shallice, T. (1979). Case-study approach in neuropsychological research. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1, 183-211. Shallice, T., Rumiati, R. I., & Zadini, A. (2000). The selective impairment of the phonological output buffer. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 517-546. Shallice, T., Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. (1983). Reading without semantics. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 111-138. Shapiro, K., & Caramazza, A. (2001). Sometimes a noun is just a noun: Comments on Bird, Howard, and Franklin (2000). Brain and Language, 76, 202-212. Shapiro, K., & Caramazza, A. (2003a)Looming a loom: Evidence for independent access to 58 grammatical and phonological properties in verb retrieval. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16, 85-112. Shapiro, K., & Caramazza, A. (2003). Grammatical processing of nouns and verbs in left frontal cortex? Neuropsychologia, 41, 1189-98. Shapiro, K., Shelton, J., & Caramazza, A. (2001). Grammatical class in lexical production and morphological processing: evidence from a case of fluent aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 665-682. Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1979). Speech errors as evidence for a serial-ordering mechanism in sentence production. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker (Eds.) Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merrill Garrett (pp. 295-341). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1992). The role of word structure in segmental serial ordering. Cognition, 42, 213-259. Stemberger, J. P. (1985). An interactive activation model of language production. In A. W. Ellis (ed.) Progress in the psychology of language (Vol. 1, pp. 143-186). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tsapkini, K., Jarema, G., & Kehayia, E. (2001). A morphological processing deficit in verbs but not in nouns: A case study in a highly inflected language. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 15, 265-288. Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Martin, R. C., & Garrett, M. F. (1999). Is “count” and “mass” information available when the noun Is not? An investigation of tip of the tongue states and anomia. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 534–558. Vousden, J. I., Brown, G. D. A., & Harley, T. A. (2000). Serial control of phonology in speech production: A hierarchical model. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 101-175. Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. A. (1987). Categories of knowledge: Further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110, 1273–1296. Warrington, E.K. & Shallice, T. (1979). Semantic access dyslexia. Brain, 102, 43-63. Wilshire, C. E. (2002). Where do aphasic phonological errors come from? Evidence from movement errors in picture naming. Aphasiology, 16, 169-197. Wilshire, C. E., & McCarthy, R. A. (1996). Experimental investigations of an impairment in phonological encoding. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 1059-1098. Wilshire, C. E., & McCarthy, R. A. (2002). Evidence for a context-sensitive word retrieval disorder in a case of nonfluent aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 165-186. Wright, J. F., & Ahmad, K. (1997). The connectionist simulation of aphasic naming. Brain and Language, 59, 367-389. Wu, D. H., Martin, R. C., & Damian, M. F. (2002). A third route for reading? Implications from a case of phonological dyslexia. Neurocase, 8, 274-295. 59 Table 1. Open theoretical issues, circa 1984; progress on these and related issues, 1984-2004. Major Issues identified in Ellis 1985 Significant progress 1984-2004 Basic architectural organization: $ Are spoken comprehension and production served by a single lexicon? $ Are world knowledge and lexical semantics distinct? $ Are word meanings and word forms represented independently? $ How are phonemic and phonetic levels distinguished? Basic architectural organization: º Open question º Open question º The independent representation of word meaning and form º Open question [ The independent representation of word form and word syntax Representation and processing in the speech output lexicon: $ Does the speech lexicon respect grammatical category distinctions? $ Are morphologically complex words represented in a decomposed manner? Representation and processing in the speech output lexicon: º Grammatical category distinctions at the level of the phonological output lexicon º Morphologically decomposed word forms [ Lexical category distinctions at the level of the phonological output lexicon Activation dynamics: $ What is the role of interactive activation in spoken word production? Specifically: $ cascading activation $ feedback $ competitive inhibition Activation dynamics: º Cascading activation from semantic-lexical-phoneme levels º Feedback from phoneme to lexical levels º Open question 60 Table 2. RGB and HW’s performance in oral and written naming. RGB HW Spoken Correct 68% 65% Semantic* 32% 26% Omissions / Unrecognizable 0% 9% Written Correct 94% 91% Semantic* 0% 0% Omissions / Unrecognizable 6% 9% *includes definitions, morphological errors and nonwords recognizable as semantic errors [skid](squid) for octopus 61 Table 3. Dante’s accuracy on forced-choice questions on trials where he could not generate the target word (N=88) (data combined from picture-naming and sentence completion tasks) Percentage Correct Grammatical gender 98 Word Length 50 First letter 53 Last letter 47 Rhyming word 48 62 Table 4. Distribution of KSR’s responses on spoken and written naming of nouns and verbs. Spoken noun Spoken verb Written noun Written verb Correct 71 89 93 55 Other word 10 6 1 14 Non-word 17 2 3 4 Omission 1 0 1 7 Morphological 1 2 2 11 Other word + morphological 0 0 0 7 63 Table 5. Percentage of total responses in SJD’s reading aloud of matched sets of affixed/unaffixed homophones. Examples of each potential error type on each list are shown in parentheses. Affixed Homophones Unaffixed Homophones Correct 50 85 Morphological error (bowled->bowl; lynx-> link) 42 0 Phonological error (frays->prays; bread->breast) 8 15 64 Figure Captions Figure 1. Ellis (1985)’s framework for speech production. Figure 2. Schematic of lemma theories of speech production (a) and the Independent Network theory of Caramazza (1997b). Figure 3. KSR’s written and spoken naming of target pictures. Figure 4. Highly discrete (a) highly interactive (b) and restricted interaction (c) accounts of spoken word production. Dotted lines and units in show activation due to semantic overlap with the target; dashed lines and units show activation due to phonological overlap with the target. (Concentric circle denote units activated by both semantic and phonological overlap.) (Greyed-out units are not directly activated by the target.) Figure 5. Framework for current theories of speech production, incorporating findings reviewed in the article. Multiple arrows between processing components denotes cascading activation; double- headed arrows indicate feedback between components. Dashed line indicates uncertainty regarding relationship between lexical semantic representations and grammatical features. Figure 6. Schematic articulatory phonological representation (gestural score) of “pan.” Articulators are shown on the left hand side. Letters on the left and the right show the association between articulatory gestures and elements in the segmental transcriptions. For each articulator, the bar represents the time in which the articulator is active. Labels within the bar refer to the degree of constriction; for some articulators, location is specified following a colon. Wide indicates that the degree of constriction is low, while closure indicates a high degree of constriction. 65 Figure 1 Conceptual Semantic System Speech Output Lexicon Phoneme Level Speech 66 Figure 2 A Semantic Representations Lemmas Phonological Representations Orthographic Representations Syntactic Representations A 67 Figure 2B Semantic Representations Phonological Representations Orthographic Representations Syntactic Representations B 68 Figure 3 69 Figure 4 A A. Highly Discrete Account CALF CAT DOG RUG SAGCAP /k/ /ae/ /t/ /g/ /z//p/ Semantic Level Lexical Level Phoneme Level 70 Figure 4 B CALF CAT DOG RUG SAGCAP /k/ /ae/ /t/ /g/ /z//p/ B. Highly Interactive Account Semantic Level Lexical Level Phoneme Level 71 Figure 4C CALF CAT DOG RUG SAGCAP /k/ /ae/ /t/ /g/ /z//p/ C. Restricted Interaction Account Semantic Level Lexical Level Phoneme Level 72 Figure 5 Lexical Semantics Speech Output Lexicon Nouns Verbs Proper Common walk +ing John book +s talk +er brain sing Grammatical Features Phonemes 73 Figure 6 VELUM Wide n TONGUE BODY ae Wide: Pharyngeal TONGUE TIP Closure: Alveolar n LIPS Closure: Labial GLOTTIS p h Wide time
File đính kèm:
- rappgoldrickcnreviewms-pdf-25183.pdf